Peter C. Gotaxche Lancet
Psychiatry Volume 1, Issue 2, July 2014, Pages 104–106
Why
I think
antidepressants cause more harm than good
In The
Lancet Psychiatry,
David Nutt and colleagues1 stated
that headlines such as “Antidepressants do more harm than good” plumb a “new
nadir in irrational polemic.” I disagree and describe here the evidence that
supports my argument so that readers can judge for themselves what they think
about the defence of these
drugs by Nutt and colleagues.
With regard to the benefits of antidepressants, in its large
meta-analysis of 100 000 patients, half of whom
were depressed, the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA)
noted that 10% more patients responded on
antidepressants than did those on placebo,2 and the Cochrane review of
depressed patients reported similar
results3 (ie,
one patient might benefit for every ten patients treated).
I believe those results were exaggerated, however, for
several reasons.4
Most
importantly, the trials were not effectively blinded. Antidepressants have conspicuous
side-effects and many patients and their doctors will therefore know whether
the blinded drug is active or placebo. A systematic review of 21 trials5 in a variety of diseases that had
both masked and non-masked outcome assessors, and which had mostly used
subjective outcomes, found that the treatment effect was
exaggerated by 36% on average
(measured as odds ratio) when non-masked observers rather than masked ones
assessed the effect. The effect of antidepressants is assessed on highly
subjective scales (eg. The Hamilton scale[1]), and if we assume that the
blinding is broken for all patients in the trials and adjust for the bias, we
will find that antidepressants have no effect (odds ratio 1·02).4
However, I do not believe that the blinding is always broken,
only that the reported effect is highly likely to
have been exaggerated. Many years ago, adequately blinded
trials of tricyclic antidepressants were done,
in which the
placebo contained atropine,
which causes dryness in the mouth like the active drugs do. These trials reported
very small, clinically
insignificant effects of tricyclic
antidepressants compared with
placebo (standardised mean difference 0·17, 95% CI 0·00–0·34).6
Another worrying finding in randomised trials is that as
many patients stop treatment on SSRIs as on placebo
for any reason.7 After only
2 months, half the patients have stopped
taking the drug.8
This finding
suggests that, overall, considering benefits and harms together, the patients
find the drugs useless.[2]
More importantly, no research shows whether these drugs work for the outcomes
that really matter, such as saving relationships
and getting people back to work.
With respect to the harms of antidepressants, most patients
who take these drugs will experience side-effects. The package inserts list
many common side-effects, of which one of the most frequent is sexual problems.
In a study9
designed to
assess this side-effect, sexual problems developed in 604 (59%) of 1022
patients who all reported no problems with sexual function before they started
using an antidepressant. The symptoms include decreased libido (50% of patients
on fluoxetine), delayed orgasm or ejaculation (also 50%), no orgasm or
ejaculation (39%), and erectile dysfunction or decreased vaginal lubrication
(22% for both combined).
Even when tapering off them slowly, half the patients have
difficulty stopping the drugs because of withdrawal
effects, which can be severe10 and long-lasting.4 We noted that withdrawal symptoms
were described in similar
terms for benzodiazepines and SSRIs and were very similar
for 37 of 42 identified symptoms.11 However, they
were not described as dependence for
SSRIs.11
To define similar problems as “dependence”
in the case
of
benzodiazepines and as “withdrawal reactions” in the case
of SSRIs is irrational. For patients, the symptoms are just the same; it can be
very hard for them to stop either type of drug.
Psychiatrists often argue, as did Nutt and colleagues,1 that
antidepressants protect against suicide. However,
I believe that no good evidence in support of this idea
exists. Good observational studies have refuted it,12 and results from randomised trials13 have shown that antidepressants are
associated with increased risk of suicide attempts (5·6 more suicide attempts per
1000 patient-years of SSRI exposure compared with placebo). Antidepressants
have not only been associated with suicide but also with homicide.4,14–16 The
FDA’s analysis2
showed that
suicidal behaviour is increased with antidepressants until about the age of 40
years—but in fact, the situation is much worse than this. Suicides and suicide
attempts were vastly underreported in the FDA’s analysis for various reasons.4 For
example, only five deaths by suicide were recorded in 52 960 patients on
antidepressants in the 2006 FDA analysis2 whereas five deaths by suicide were recorded in 2963
patients on paroxetine alone in a meta-analysis from 1993.17
SSRIs are particularly harmful for elderly patients. Results
from a carefully controlled cohort study18 of people older than 65 years of age with depression showed
that SSRIs led to falls more often than did older antidepressants or if the
depression was left untreated. For every
28 elderly people treated for 1 year with an
SSRI, there was one additional death, compared with no treatment.18 SSRIs have also stimulant effects
and might precipitate conversion to bipolar disorder in about 10% of children
aged 10–14 years under the care of mental health services .19
SSRIs are very poor drugs and I doubt they are safe at any
age. The first SSRI was fluoxetine, which the German drug regulator deemed
“totally unsuitable for the treatment of depression”.14,20 I, and others,4,21 have written about the controversy
surrounding this drug and the process by which it nevertheless came to be
approved and widely used. I have written previously4 that there has been heavy marketing
and widespread crime committed by drug companies, including fraud, illegal
promotion, and corruption of psychiatrists. In the USA, psychiatrists receive
more money from the drug industry than any other specialty.4,22 As a result, enough antidepressants
are prescribed every year in Denmark to provide treatment for every person in
the country for 6 years of their lives.4
I
believe this situation is not sound and that it also partly portrays the fact
that many patients cannot stop these drugs because of intolerable withdrawal
symptoms.
SSRIs have been shown to have minimal or nonexistent benefit
in patients with mild or moderate depression23 and I think they might not even work for severe
depression.4
They should
be used very sparingly, if at all, and always with a clear plan for tapering
off them. The so-called maintenance
studies, in which patients after successful treatment get randomly assigned to
continue with the drug or a placebo, cannot be interpreted as showing that the
patients still need the drug because withdrawal symptoms, which can include depression,
are inflicted on the placebo group.
Nutt and two of his co-authors, Guy M Goodwin and Stephen
Lawrie, have between them declared 22
conflicts of interest in relation to drug companies.1 I wonder whether this declaration
explains their dismissal of psychotherapy (although it is effective and
recommended by NICE) and their description of my evidence-based views as a
somewhat irrational polemic that is insulting to the discipline of psychiatry
and is reinforcing stigma against mental illnesses. They also talk about
anti-psychiatry, anti-capitalism, and a conspiracy theory. This is the language of
people who are short of arguments.
Peter C
Gøtzsche
Nordic
Cochrane Centre, Dept 7811 Rigshospitalet,
Copenhagen, Denmark
pcg@cochrane.dk
I
declare no competing interests
1
Nutt DJ, Goodwin GM, Bhugra D, Fazel S, Lawrie S. Attacks on antidepressants:
signs of deep-seated stigma? Lancet
Psychiatry 2014
; 1:
103–04.
2
Laughren TP. Overview for December 13 Meeting of psychopharmacologic drugs
advisory committee (PDAC). 2006, Nov 16. http://www.fda.gov/
ohrms/dockets/ac/06/briefi ng/2006-4272b1-01-FDA.pdf (accessed Oct 22, 2012).
3
Arroll B, Elley CR, Fishman T, et al. Antidepressants versus placebo for
depression in primary care. Cochrane
Database Syst Rev 2009; 3:
CD007954.
4
Gøtzsche PC. Deadly medicines and organised crime: how big pharma has corrupted
health care. London: Radcliff e Publishing, 2013
5
Hróbjartsson A, Thomsen AS, Emanuelsson F, et al. Observer bias in randomised
clinical trials with binary outcomes: systematic review of trials with both
blinded and non-blinded outcome assessors. BMJ
2012;
344:
e1119.
6
Moncrieff J, Wessely S, Hardy R. Active placebos versus antidepressants for
depression. Cochrane
Database Syst Rev 2004; 1: CD003012.
7
Barbui C, Furukawa TA, Cipriani A. Effectiveness of paroxetine in the treatment
of acute major depression in adults: a systematic reexamination of published
and unpublished data from randomized trials. CMAJ
2008;
178:
296–305.
8
Serna MC, Cruz I, Real J, et al. Duration and adherence of antidepressant
treatment (2003 to 2007) based on prescription database. Eur Psychiatry 2010;
25:
206–13.
9
Montejo A, Llorca G, Izquierdo J, et al. Incidence of sexual dysfunction
associated with antidepressant agents: a prospective multicenter study of 1022
outpatients. Spanish Working Group for the study of psychotropic related
sexual
dysfunction. J
Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62 (suppl 3): 10–21.
10
Fava GA, Bernardi M, Tomba E, et al. Effects of gradual discontinuation of
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in panic disorder with agoraphobia.Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2007; 10: 835–38.
11
Nielsen M, Hansen EH, Gøtzsche PC. What is the difference between dependence
and withdrawal reactions? A comparison of benzodiazepines and selective
serotonin re-uptake inhibitors. Addiction
2012;
107:
900–08.
12
Zahl PH, De Leo D, Ekeberg Ø, et al. The relationship between sales of SSRI,
TCA and suicide rates in the Nordic
countries. BMC Psychiatry 2010; 10: 62. 13 Fergusson D, Doucette S, Glass KC, et al.
Association between suicide attempts and selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors: systematic review of randomised controlled trials. BMJ 2005;
330:
396.
14
Healy D. Let Them Eat Prozac. New York: New York University Press; 2004.
15
Moore TJ, Glenmullen J, Furberg CD. Prescription drugs associated with reports
of violence towards others. PLoS
One 2010;
5:
e15337.
16
Lucire Y, Crotty C. Antidepressant-induced akathisia-related homicides associated
with diminishing mutations in metabolizing genes of the CYP450 family. Pharmgenomics Pers Med 2011;
4:
65–81.
17
Montgomery SA, Dunner DL, Dunbar GC. Reduction of suicidal thoughts with
paroxetine in comparison with reference antidepressants and placebo. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1995; 5: 5–13. 18 Coupland C,
Dhiman P, Morriss R, et al.
Antidepressant use and risk of adverse outcomes in older people: population
based cohort study. BMJ
2011;
343:
d4551.
19
Martin A, Young C, Leckman JF, et al. Age eff ects on antidepressant-induced manic
conversion. Arch
Pediatr Adolesc Med 2004; 158: 773–80.
20
Internal Eli Lilly memo. Bad Homburg. 1984 May 25 (available on request).
21
Virapen J. Side eff ects: death. College Station: Virtualbookworm.com Publishing,
2010.
22
Insel TR. Psychiatrists’ relationships with pharmaceutical companies: part of
the problem or part of the solution? JAMA
2010;
303:
1192–93.
23
Fournier JC, DeRubeis RJ, Hollon SD, et al. Antidepressant drug effects and
depression severity: a patient-level meta-analysis. JAMA 2010;303,
47-53.
[1] It
is much worse than Gotzsche claims. For
one thing Prof. Irving Kirsch has researched breaking blinds for SSRIs, and
found it to be over 85%for both doctors and patients. “The Emperor’s New Drugs,
p13, Secondly the Hamilton Depression
Rating Scale (HDRS) consists of 17 question, of which often only 13 are
used. Of those used three are for
sleeping better, (early night, mid-night, and early morning). Since SSRIs are
tranquilizers (cause people
to sleep more) there will be 23% benefit based on those 3 questions.
[2] This again is a major
place for distorting
the results. The standard practice (not
mentioned in the journal articles) is that of last observation is carried
forward. The last observation before the
patient dropped out is carried to the end of the study thus inflating the
number of patients who completed the study and deflating side effects which
aren’t reported by the patient who dropped out. See Ben Goldacre’s Bad Pharma,
p 69.